Home Now You See It, But Would You Later? Examining the Mechanisms of Satisfaction of Search in the Fatigued Radiologist
Post
Cancel

Now You See It, But Would You Later? Examining the Mechanisms of Satisfaction of Search in the Fatigued Radiologist

Radiologists are increasingly being challenged to do more with less: to care for more patients, to cover more hours of the day, to issue more detailed reports simultaneously, and to provide care at more sites . The higher workload has increased radiologists’ fatigue as well as the potential for committing diagnostic errors. Fatigue comes in many forms: physical, emotional, and cognitive. Given the nature of the interpretive task, visual fatigue is yet one more important form to consider in radiology .

Fatigue has been shown to contribute to errors in radiological diagnosis by decreasing radiologists’ accuracy . Over the past few decades, Dr. Krupinski et al. have contributed a great deal to the literature on factors that affect radiologists’ perception during the interpretive task. Krupinski et al. have shown that, after a day of clinical reading, there is a statistically significant decrease in the area under the ROC curve for identifying fractures on musculoskeletal radiographs , as well as pulmonary nodules on chest CT . In both studies, quantitative estimates of oculomotor strain and subjective assessments of fatigue were also higher after the clinical workday. Radiologists spent less time interpreting CT colonography exams later in the day as levels of fatigue rose . Residents at one institution had significantly higher rates of discrepancy with the final attending review during the last 2 hours of a 12-hour shift compared to the preceding 10 hours .

Cognitive errors in radiology include anchoring bias, framing bias, availability bias, and errors of perception, such as satisfaction of search . Satisfaction of search is a common type of perceptual error . It is the proverbial gorilla in the radiograph, but, instead of missing it entirely, we fixate on it and miss other, sometimes more subtle findings . Berbaum et al. demonstrated that satisfaction of search associated with presentation of a simulated pulmonary nodule on film-based chest radiographs resulted in a statistically significant decrease in the area under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve . Samuel et al. used gaze tracking to show that simulated pulmonary nodules only received prolonged visual attention in the absence of native abnormalities on the film-based chest radiograph .

Prior studies using film radiographs interpreted on a light box postulated that satisfaction of search was due to decreased accuracy, specifically defined as a change in the height of the ROC curve . However, more recent studies from Dr. Krupinski’s group using digital images viewed on a workstation suggest that satisfaction of search is in fact secondary to a shift in the decision threshold, reflected by a change in both true positive fractions and false positive fractions . This type of change is noted to move points along the ROC curve, but not to shift the height of the curve itself. And despite the wealth of literature on the effects of fatigue—a system-based error—and satisfaction of search—a cognitive error—on the interpretative process in radiology, few studies have examined the interaction of the two effects . Dr. Krupinski et al. designed their latest study to specifically examine the mechanism of satisfaction of search in the setting of fatigue.

Satisfaction of search produced a threshold shift in the ROC curves for both faculty and resident readers, with a decrease in median true and false positive fractions. The authors interpret this as a decreased willingness to identify an abnormality. The threshold shifts were significantly more severe for residents than for the faculty in the satisfaction of search condition (ie, when a simulated pulmonary nodule was present). Interpretation times were not significantly different between the satisfaction of search and non-satisfaction of search conditions; a native abnormality was observed to require more interpretation time.

The authors have decades of experience in conducting perception research and, in particular, in studying the mechanisms of satisfaction of search. A robust study design was used, including standardized viewing conditions, a mix of faculty and trainee readers, a wide variety of chest radiography diagnoses, a randomization of case presentations, a counterbalanced design, and an appropriate washout period between reading sessions. However, one factor that was nonuniform was the degree of fatigue experienced by the readers before each session. All readers presented for study-related interpretation sessions after having completed at least an 8-hour clinical workday. However, the authors acknowledge that just over half of the readers spent the day interpreting only one type of modality, whereas the rest interpreted two or more types. Although this finding implies an inherent difference in the number and the type of clinical case interpreted before each study reading session, all clinical shifts were considered equivalent in terms of the degree of fatigue induced. It is realistic to wonder if some of the readers were in fact more fatigued than others because of a busier shift, recent on-call duties, or non–work-related factors. Unlike in prior work from this group, each reader’s degree of fatigue was neither subjectively nor objectively characterized before a study reading shift.

Get Radiology Tree app to read full this article<

Get Radiology Tree app to read full this article<

Get Radiology Tree app to read full this article<

Get Radiology Tree app to read full this article<

References

  • 1. Bhargavan M., Kaye A.H., Forman H.P., et. al.: Workload of radiologists in United States in 2006–2007 and trends since 1991–1992. Radiology 2009; 252:

  • 2. McDonald R.J., Schwartz K.M., Eckel L.J., et. al.: The effects of changes in utilization and technological advancements of cross-sectional imaging on radiologist workload. Acad Radiol 2010; 22: pp. 1191-1198.

  • 3. Reiner B.I., Krupinski E.: The insidious problem of fatigue in medical imaging practice. J Digit Imaging 2012; 25: pp. 3-6.

  • 4. Ruutiainen A.T., Durand D.J., Scanlon M.H., et. al.: Increased error rates in preliminary reports issued by radiology residents working more than 10 consecutive hours overnight. Acad Radiol 2013; 20: pp. 305-311.

  • 5. Burling D., Halligan S., Altman D.G., et. al.: CT colonography interpretation times: effect of reader experience, fatigue, and scan findings in a multi-centre setting. Eur Radiol 2006; 16: pp. 1745-1749.

  • 6. Krupinski E.A., Berbaum K.S., Caldwell R.T., et. al.: Long radiology workdays reduce detection and accommodation accuracy. J Am Coll Radiol 2010; 7: pp. 698-704.

  • 7. Krupinski E., Reiner B.I.: Real-time occupational stress and fatigue measurement in medical imaging practice. J Digit Imaging 2012; 25: pp. 319-324.

  • 8. Krupinski E.A., Berbaum K.S., Caldwell R.T., et. al.: Do long radiology workdays affect nodule detection in dynamic CT interpretation?. J Am Coll Radiol 2012; 9: pp. 191-198.

  • 9. Lee C.S., Nagy P.G., Weaver S.J., et. al.: Cognitive and system factors contributing to diagnostic errors in radiology. Am J Roentgenol 2013; 201: pp. 611-617.

  • 10. Samuel S., Kundel L., Nodine F., et. al.: Mechanism of satisfaction of search: eye position recordings in the reading of chest radiographs. Radiology 1995; 194: pp. 895-902.

  • 11. Fitzgerald R.: Error in radiology. Clin Radiol 2001; 56: pp. 938-946.

  • 12. Drew T., Võ M.L., Wolfe J.M.: The invisible gorilla strikes again: sustained inattentional blindness in expert observers. Psychol Sci 2013; 24: pp. 1848-1853.

  • 13. Berbaum K.S., Franken E.A., Dorfman D.D., et. al.: Satisfaction of search in diagnostic radiology. Invest Radiol 1990; 25: pp. 133-140.

  • 14. Berbaum K.S., Krupinski E.A., Schartz K.M., et. al.: Satisfaction of search in chest radiography 2015. Acad Radiol 2015; 22: pp. 1457-1465.

  • 15. Moskowitz P.S.: Gathering storm clouds suggest the need for a culture change in radiology: radiologist-centered imaging. Radiology 2015; 276: pp. 3-7.

  • 16. Harolds J.A., Parikh J.R., Bluth E.I., et. al.: Burnout of radiologists: frequency, risk factors, and remedies: a report of the ACR commission on human resources. J Am Coll Radiol 2016; 13: pp. 411-416.

  • 17. Nicola R., McNeeley M.F., Bhargava P.: Burnout in radiology. Curr Probl Diagn Radiol 2015; 44: pp. 389-390.

  • 18. McNeeley M.F., Perez F.A., Chew F.S.: The emotional wellness of radiology trainees: prevalence and predictors of burnout. Acad Radiol 2013; 20: pp. 647-655.

  • 19. Chew F.S., Mulcahy M.J., Porrino J.A., et. al.: Prevalence of burnout among musculoskeletal radiologists. Skeletal Radiol 2017; 46: pp. 497-506.

  • 20. Kruskal J.B., Berkowitz S., Geis J.R., et. al.: Big data and machine learning-strategies for driving this bus: a summary of the 2016 intersociety summer conference. J Am Coll Radiol 2017; 14: pp. 811-817.

  • 21. Kohli M., Prevedello L.M., Filice R.W., et. al.: Implementing machine learning in radiology practice and research. Am J Roentgenol 2017; 208: pp. 754-760.

This post is licensed under CC BY 4.0 by the author.